Looking for "The Face Within the Face" in Man
[Article
from New York Times
(1906)]
 |
(Top down) Brain of Helmholtz (After Photo of Cast by Hausemann). Brain of Papuan from British New Guinea; Specimen in Anatomical
Laboratory at Columbia University (Bottom) Brain of Gorilla (D. 658,
Mus. Roy. Coll. Surgeons of England.) |
Dr.
Spitzka, Surrounded by His Collection of Brains,
Discusses the
Latest Phases of Cerebral Science —
No Such Thing as a "Criminal Brain
Type," He Says.
“IF you have brains, prepare to shed them now.” If
one may paraphrase Mark Antony's famous invocation thus, it would give
voice to one of the latest demands that the scientific investigator is
making to those who would contribute something to the search for
accurate knowledge on a subject of vital importance to humanity. In
that spheroidal box of bone, the top of which is adorned, in some
instances, with hair, that every man, woman, and child carries above
his or her shoulders, there is usually a soft gray and white substance,
full of wrinkles and creases, weighing on an average, fifty ounces,
called a brain. It is this soft substance that directs the thinking and
moving machinery called man - and how it does it, and why it does it
well in some cases and badly in others is the problem science would
solve. Hence the need for brain specimens to furnish the brain
laboratory of the investigator in this curious department of knowledge.
Three associations have already been formed for the preservation of
brains after death for the benefit of scientific research - the Mutual
Autopsy Society of Paris, the American Anthropometric Society, and the
Cornell Brain Association. Quite recently, also, the Association of
American Anatomists appointed a committee on brain bequests and
preservation methods, consisting of Dr. Edward Anthony Spitzka, Prof.
Wilder of Cornell University and Dr. Hrdlicka. The object of this
committee is to suggest ways for making bequests that will hold in law,
and to bring about a uniform method to be used in brain dissection.
On the top floor of a handsome modern dwelling in this city, 66 East
Seventy-third Street, is the workshop of one of the recognized leaders
in this department of inquiry, Dr. Edward Anthony Spitzka, whose
father, Dr. E. C. Spitzka, the noted alienist, living in the same
house, is the discover of the inter-optic lobes of the lower brain. To
the uninitiated this workshop of the Spitzkas is somewhat forbidding, a
medley of charts covered with unintelligible lines and measurements,
learned books and pamphlets, death masks, physiological casts, and, the
key to much of this mystery, jar upon jar of human brains - brains of
murderers, brains of scientists, brains of writers, brains of plain,
ordinary people, brains of apes. There are about thirty in all, each in
its own jar, swimming in a solution of formalin and salt. At the
College of Physicians and Surgeons there are 600 more specimens of the
same kind in Dr. Spitzka's care, together with brains of the lower
animals. Each brain in the doctor's private study has a record of its
life history, an account of what it did, for weal or woe, while it had
a body to serve its behests, before it reached its last abode, an
inert, pulpy mass, in the formalin jar.
“Yes, there are the brains of some noted criminals in some of
those jars,” remarked Dr. Spitzka, in answer to my inquiry,
“and. if certain theories as to the ‘criminal brain
type’ were correct, it ought to be possible to distinguish
the jar that holds the criminal brain from its neighbor that is
occupied by the brain of some man of genius. But I have yet to find the
scientist who could make such a discrimination. Notwithstanding a
notion that in some quarters is rather prevalent, the study of
criminals by anthropological methods does not necessarily tend to
establish a criminal ‘type’ nor to describe crime
as equivalent to infantilism, epilepsy, atavism, genius, or anything
else maintained by the Lombroso school. Many criminals show not a
single anomaly in their physical or mental make-up, while many persons
with marked evidences of structural aberration have never exhibited
the. criminal tendency. Every attempt to prove crime to be due to a
constitution peculiar only to criminals has failed signally. It is
because most criminals are drawn from the ranks of the low, the
degraded, the outcast, that investigators were ever deceived into
attempting to set up a ‘type’ of criminal. The
social conditions which foster the great majority of crimes are more
needful or study and improvement; by such means only can crime be made
less prevalent – among the sane, at least. Society wrongs
neither the criminal nor itself if criminals were studied as
individuals and not as a ‘species.’ From the study
of known normal brains we have learned that there is a certain range of
variation of structural characters. No two brains are exactly alike,
and the greatest source of error in the assertions of Benedickt and
Lombroso has been the finding of this or that variation in a criminal's
brain and maintaining such to be a characteristic of the
‘criminal constitution’ - unmindful of the fact
that like variations of structure may and do exist in the brains of
normal, moral persons. It were as justifiable to set up a
‘criminal brain type’ as to speak of an
‘insane brain type’; and the classes of criminality
are quite as numerous as the kinds of insanity. In criminals
demonstrably sane in life I have failed thus far to find any cerebral
characteristic ascribable to an alleged ‘criminal
constitution.’ Out of the brain specimens of seventeen
murderers that I have studied, some of which are in the jars that you
see, I have been unable to find one showing anything abnormal, or that
could be taken in any way to be corroborative of a criminal brain type.
“Take the case of Czolgosz, for instance, whose brain I
examined - although I did not preserve it. So far as our knowledge of
the correlation of brain structure and brain function extends, nothing
was found in the brain of this assassin that would condone his crime
for the reason of mental disease due to intrinsic cerebral defect or
distortion. The brain weight, a little over rather than under the
average, pointed to a good condition of the organ. The development or
the fissures was in the direction usual in ordinary, average brains.
Generally speaking, the brain did not exhibit that especial kind of
symmetry of structure in the cerebral halves that is so characteristic
of the brains of highly endowed individuals; but there were absolutely
none of those conditions present that could have been at the bottom of
any abnormality or mental derangement. . Hence, in Czoigosz's case, the
verdict must be, ‘socially diseased and perverted, but not
mentally diseased.’
“If there is error in the ‘criminal type’
theory what is there in the claims of phrenology? you ask. Absolutely
nothing. The idea that the bumps or depressions on a man's head
indicate the presence or absence of certain moral characteristics in
his mental equipment is one
of the absurdities developed from studies in this field that has long
since been discarded by science. The ideas of the phrenologist Gall,
however ridiculous they may now seem in the light of a century's
progress, were nevertheless destined to become metamorphosed into the
modern principles of cerebral localization. This is now a firmly
established doctrine, amplified and systematized by contributions from
many sources, and important results should be expected from studies of
brains of the various races, sexes, and individuals with marked mental
traits. From this study we know that persons possessing great
intellectual capacity, persons excelling in the creative arts, the
sciences, and in human affairs in general are apt to have heavier,
larger brains than those of the ordinary population, as well as brains
showing a greater complexity in their convolutions. As you can see in
these specimens, the brain surface presents alternating depressions or
fissures marking these convolutions. The surface pattern at these
fissures and, convolutions shows the same general features in all
normal human brains, and, to some extent, in the higher anthropoid apes
as well. When we come to make more careful comparisons, however, and
delve more deeply into details of the arrangement of these fissures, we
recognize many differences, not only in the brains of races and of
individuals, but also in the two cerebral halves of the same
individual. It is a significant fact, by the way, that we find the
higher anthropoids not further removed from the lower races of man with
respect to brain weight and structure than are the latter from a number
of men or superior intellect in the white race. The jump in this
respect from a Cuvier or a Thackeray, for instance, to a Zulu or a
Bushman is no less than from the latter to the gorilla or the orang.
“There is a physiognomy of brain which portrays
intellectuality quite as often as does the outward physiognomy. That
this so-called cerebral physiognomy is difficult to describe in so many
words, or that we are occasionally deceived by it, does not alter the
fact that we may generally learn to recognize and judge it. Patient
study of a greater amount of suitable material may in the future enable
us to express in scientific terms what is now a mere personal
conviction. In these studies, by the way, one cannot restrain the
thought, What a pity that we have not the brain of a Newton, a
Shakespeare, a Michael Angelo, a Beethoven, or an Edgar Allan Poe! How
much more useful would be the contemplation of such organs of thought,
how elevating, how inspiring would their lessons be! Hitherto, however,
we have examined, almost exclusively, the brains of pauper
ne'er-do-wells and criminals.

(Left) Frontal Aspect of the Brain of a
Papuan, a Native of British New Guinea
(Right) Frontal Aspect of the Brain of George Francis Train. This is in
many respects One of the Best Developed Brains on Record.
"One of the best-developed brains on record recently came into my
collection, this brain that you see here. To whom did it belong? He was
a familiar figure to every old New Yorker, and his name is probably
known from here to the Pacific. In weight this brain of his is above
the average, and in structural features admirable, in the depth and
complexity of the fissures and convolutions with which it is marked.
Contrast it with this Papuan brain. Can you not recognize at a glance
the intellectual physiognomy in the one, and the lack of it in the
other? I think any student, without knowing anything about the man from
whom it was taken, would say that this brain belonged to a person of
note, of unusual attainments and ability, and he certainly was a
remarkable man, even if there may have been some little defect
somewhere in his mentality - although his brain shows not a single
imperfection to the anatomist. He had an eventful career. Starting out
a penniless lad, at the age of 20 he had accumulated an income of
$10,000. He introduced the first street railway in Europe. In this
country he built the Union Pacific Railroad and opened up navigation
and commerce between the United States and the Far East. More than once
he got into jail, and in six different courts, without proper trial, he
was adjudged a lunatic, although no insanity expert would sign his
commitment papers. Just before he died, not much more than a year ago,
an old man at the Mills Hotel, he dictated his remarkable
autobiography, an amazing record of facts, accurate in detail,
admirable in expression - a book of about 100,000 words in length, all
of which was dictated in the space of thirty-five hours. Possibly, in
some little fibre of this wonderful brain of his, there was what we
might call a ‘screw loose,’ though I have been
unable to detect it. Did you ever see him on his old bench in Madison
Square playing with the children? He was a strange, many-sided
character, an encyclopedia of information, and the aspect, the face
written, on his brain, complex and highly developed as you see, denotes
just about what he was, the eccentric ‘citizen’
George Francis Train.
(L
to R) Cross
Sections of the Brains of the Cat, Baboon, and Man, Taken at
Approximately the Same Plane
and Drawn of About the Same Size, to
Better Show the Relatively Greater Mass of White Matter in the Human
Brain.
“In comparing brains, such as this specimen from the head of
George Francis Train, with others lower down in the scale of
intelligence, besides the differences in weight and structure, you will
notice a difference in composition, and in a direction that overturns a
popular conception. One often hears it said of an intellectual man,
‘He has lots of gray matter.’ To be sure the
statement is true enough, but not in the sense commonly implied. The
brain of man is characterized more by its preponderance of white matter
over that of gray matter than for its preponderance of gray matter over
the gray matter of the lower animals, excluding perhaps the larger
whales and the elephants, The value of arriving at a true estimate of
the importance of the ratio of white and gray matter has been greatly
enhanced by recent researches, and, as a result, it is seen that the
preponderance of white over gray matter indicates the higher
intelligence.
“An interesting and most important branch of the study of
brain anatomy is the comparison of racial brains. In spite of the
meagre records in this direction up to the present time, there seems to
be little doubt of the existence of typical distinctions between the
brains of different races. That such distinguishing features have not
yet been definitely established is due entirely to the lack of
sufficient specimens. For a while, at least, we must be content to say
that there is something in the ‘cerebral
physiognomy’ which seems at times quite as expressive, in the
brains of the inferiorly equipped races when compared with those of the
highly intellectual as the dull, stupid, vacuous, outer physiognomy of
the half-witted individual differs from the bright, animated, forceful,
and energetic look in the face of the vigorous thinker and talented
genius. In this racial brain study, the outer cerebral appearances of
the Eskimo, for example, differ from those of the white man, and both
of these differ radically from that of the Papuan.
“In this branch of the study it may be of interest to note
that I have just received the brain of a native of Van Diemen's Land,
the first specimen of the kind to reach a scientific investigator. The
race is small of stature - you remember Conan Doyle has one of them in
‘The Sign of the Four’ - and, the brain, as you
see, is small and very well marked. I have not as yet had time to make
a complete dissection of it. Such investigations should be carried on
not only among the purer races, but among the mixing races as well. And
nowhere in the world is the mixture of races going on so actively as in
this country. If we may judge from the present indications of the
formation of an American family of the Aryan race, the conditions
governing the population of this continent seem to have been peculiarly
advantageous to the preservation and restoration of the best types,
characterized by greater energy and culture. We may therefore expect to
find bigger and better brains here, and researches of this kind should
be encouraged by a greater accumulation of the requisite material.
“That is the great difficulty with which we have to contend
in pursuing this study - the difficulty in procuring enough brain
specimens. The very suggestion of an autopsy is looked upon with horror
by most people; yet it is a sentimentality prejudicial to all progress
in this line of scientific research. An anatomical examination of this
kind, conducted with expert hands, no more violates respect for the
body of the deceased than does the embalming process. With our modern
perfected methods there is no mutilation of the body in an autopsy.
Throughout the history of medicine knowledge of disease has been
obtained only through the study of processes and the results of disease
as revealed after death. When this is understood by a greater number of
people, not only medicine, but anatomy and anthropology as well, will
reap a benefit: which in its turn will surely be useful to the world."
From
the New York Times,
4 Mar 1906, SM page 3.
See also:
Today in Science History, birthdate entry for Edward Anthony Spitzka on 17 Jun 1876.